Stories are Software

Stories are Software

If you heard stories of loss and loss and loss and grief and loss and guilt and shame and sadness and loss, this would have an effect on your values, beliefs, nervous system, limbic system, reproductive system, stereo system...

I feel bad so lets put on depressing music when going through hard times so that stressful emotions can do more damage please.

So the stereo system gets effected when you hear stories of loss.

Focusing on loser stories wires the brain to get used to losing and to not flexing the 'winning muscles' in the brain.

Winning muscles?

If a person wins and loses at least they know what winning feels like.  They can get excited at the idea of winning again.  So the state of excitement is a practiced state of being or a winning muscle.
Dopamine!  Endorphins!  Yes!!!

On the other hand, if there is only losing stories, then there is no excitement.

There aren't any feel good states circulating.

Eventually this state of guilt, sadness, shame etc becomes further habituated.

We can become very skilled at feeling bad just from all the practicing!

The real kicker is that the brain becomes wired to feel bad more often than not and can't get to a feel good state for any sustainable time.  This is called anhedonia.

'Anhedonia' is the inability to experience pleasure in things normally rewarding.
'It's also a cardinal symptom of depression' - Aaron Heller

This was discovered in the research that led to the conclusion that 'depression saps endurance of the brain's reward circuitry.

Very bad!

circuiting
Bad brain, why you do that?

It was those stories of loss and worry cycling through over and over.
The stories we tell ourselves have an effect on the brain and consequently our behaviour!

Fortunately, software is re-writeable.

The brain has the ability to rewire itself in response to experience.
This is something you can use to your benefit or your detriment and everyone is doing it every day.  So how to give your brain the experiential learning that allows neuroplasticity to rewire your brain in better ways?

It has a large part with what stories you are telling yourself and how they end.

The stereo system sounds amazing now that the music is different.

And they danced.

Fin.

 

 

Meditation for Cultivating Resilience

Meditation for Cultivating Resilience

Richie Davidson is a neuroscientist and author of 'The Emotional Life of Your Brain', that decided to study the brain and in particular, happiness.  His work has recently been shown in several documentary films and he has also been a speaker with his holiness the Dalai Lama about meditation and neuroscience.

In this clip we can hear Richie talking about meditation and how it can change the brain to allow people to become more resilient.

"I have been studying the brain mechanisms that underlie emotions and variations among people in emotion.  And as part of this research we have been very interested in how we can transform our emotions in ways that are more beneficial to make people happier, to make them more resilient, to improve their well being, and to improve their health more generally."

One of the things that is striking when you look at humanity is the extraordinary diversity among people  and how they respond to the world around them.  How they respond to life's slings and arrows, how they respond to adversity.  Adversity happens to everyone.  It's something we can't avoid.  And what distinguishes among people is their emotional response to these kinds of challenges that they experience and over the last ten years in particular, we have been very interested in the role of meditation in the cultivation of more positive emotional qualities and the cultivation of resilience. "

'And so we have been interested in exploring how meditation changes the brain and how these changes in the brain may be responsible for and accompany changes that occur in consciousness, emotional reactions, in bodily processes for our physical health.  So we've been engaged in a very broad program of research to better understand how meditation practices can produce these kinds of beneficial changes.'  Richie Davidson Ph.D.

Even though there have been mental training strategies that have been extraordinary for helping to be more resilient, it is interesting to see what the next step is now that science can measure what is happening in the brain, how to change it and how to use advances in technology to merge all this together in the field of personal development.

Military Funds Brain-Computer Interfaces to Control Feelings

A $70 million program will try to develop brain implants able to regulate emotions in the mentally ill.

Brain reader: An array of micro-electrodes printed on plastic can record from the brain’s surface. It is 6.5 millimeters on a side.

Researcher Jose Carmena has worked for years training macaque monkeys to move computer cursors and robotic limbs with their minds. He does so by implanting electrodes into their brains to monitor neural activity. Now, as part of a sweeping $70 million program funded by the U.S. military, Carmena has a new goal: to use brain implants to read, and then control, the emotions of mentally ill people.

This week the Defense Advanced Research Projects Agency, or DARPA, awarded two large contracts to Massachusetts General Hospital and the University of California, San Francisco, to create electrical brain implants capable of treating seven psychiatric conditions, including addiction, depression, and borderline personality disorder.

The project builds on expanding knowledge about how the brain works; the development of microlectronic systems that can fit in the body; and substantial evidence that thoughts and actions can be altered with well-placed electrical impulses to the brain.

“Imagine if I have an addiction to alcohol and I have a craving,” says Carmena, who is a professor at the University of California, Berkeley, and involved in the UCSF-led project. “We could detect that feeling and then stimulate inside the brain to stop it from happening.”

The U.S. faces an epidemic of mental illness among veterans, including suicide rates three or four times that of the general public. But drugs and talk therapy are of limited use, which is why the military is turning to neurological devices, says Justin Sanchez, manager of the DARPA program, known as Subnets, for Systems-Based Neurotechnology for Emerging Therapies.

“We want to understand the brain networks [in] neuropsychiatric illness, develop technology to measure them, and then do precision signaling to the brain,” says Sanchez. “It’s something completely different and new. These devices don’t yet exist.”

Under the contracts, which are the largest awards so far supporting President Obama’s BRAIN Initiative, the brain-mapping program launched by the White House last year, UCSF will receive as much as $26 million and Mass General up to $30 million. Companies including the medical device giant Medtronic and startup Cortera Neurotechnologies, a spin-out from UC Berkeley’s wireless laboratory, will supply technology for the effort. Initial research will be in animals, but DARPA hopes to reach human tests within two or three years.

The research builds on a small but quickly growing market for devices that work by stimulating nerves, both inside the brain and outside it. More than 110,000 Parkinson’s patients have received deep-brain stimulators built by Medtronic that control body tremors by sending electric pulses into the brain. More recently, doctors have used such stimulators to treat severe cases of obsessive-compulsive disorder (see “Brain Implants Can Reset Misfiring Circuits”). Last November, the U.S. Food & Drug Administration approved NeuroPace, the first implant that both records from the brain and stimulates it (see “Zapping Seizures Away”). It is used to watch for epileptic seizures and then stop them with electrical pulses. Altogether, U.S. doctors bill for about $2.6 billion worth of neural stimulation devices a year, according to industry estimates.

Researchers say they are making rapid improvements in electronics, including small, implantable computers. Under its program, Mass General will work with Draper Laboratories in Cambridge, Massachusetts, to develop new types of stimulators. The UCSF team is being supported by microelectronics and wireless researchers at UC Berkeley, who have created several prototypes of miniaturized brain implants. Michel Maharbiz, a professor in Berkeley’s electrical engineering department, says the Obama brain initiative, and now the DARPA money, has created a “feeding frenzy” around new technology. “It’s a great time to do tech for the brain,” he says.

The new line of research has been dubbed “affective brain-computer interfaces” by some, meaning electronic devices that alter feelings, perhaps under direct control of a patient’s thoughts and wishes. “Basically, we’re trying to build the next generation of psychiatric brain stimulators,” says Alik Widge, a researcher on the Mass General team.

Darin Dougherty, a psychiatrist who directs Mass General’s division of neurotherapeutics, says one aim could be to extinguish fear in veterans with post-traumatic stress disorder, or PTSD. Fear is generated in the amygdala—a part of the brain involved in emotional memories. But it can be repressed by signals in another region, the ventromedial pre-frontal cortex. “The idea would be to decode a signal in the amygdala showing overactivity, then stimulate elsewhere to [suppress] that fear,” says Dougherty.

Such research isn’t without ominous overtones. In the 1970s, Yale University neuroscientist Jose Delgado showed he could cause people to feel emotions, like relaxation or anxiety, using implants he called “stimoceivers.” But Delgado, also funded by the military, left the U.S. after Congressional hearings in which he was accused of developing “totalitarian” mind-control devices. According to scientists funded by DARPA, the agency has been anxious about how the Subnets program could be perceived, and it has appointed an ethics panel to oversee the research.
Psychiatric implants would in fact control how mentally ill people act, although in many cases indirectly, by changing how they feel. For instance, a stimulator that stops a craving for cocaine would alter an addict’s behavior. “It’s to change what people feel and to change what they do. Those are intimately tied,” says Dougherty.

Dougherty says a brain implant would only be considered for patients truly debilitated by mental illness, and who can’t be helped with drugs and psychotherapy. “This is never going to be a first-line option: ‘Oh, you have PTSD, let’s do surgery,’ ” says Dougherty. “It’s going to be for people who don’t respond to the other treatments.”

DARPA article : http://www.technologyreview.com/news/527561/military-funds-brain-computer-interfaces-to-control-feelings/

 

References for the Ten Misconceptions of Memory Reconsolidation

References for the Ten Misconceptions of Memory Reconsolidation

References

Agren, T. (2014). Human reconsolidation: A reactivation and update. Brain Research Bulletin, 105, 70– 82. doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2013.12.010

Alexander, F., & French, T. M. (1946). Psychoanalytic therapy: Principles and application. New York, NY: Ronald Press.

Baker, K. D., McNally, G. P., & Richardson, R. (2013). Memory retrieval before or after extinction reduces recovery of fear in adolescent rats. Learning & Memory, 20, 467–473. doi:10.1101/lm.031989.113

Balderas, I., Rodriguez-Ortiz, C. J., & BermudezRattoni, F. (2013). Retrieval and reconsolidation of object recognition memory are independent processes in the perirhinal cortex. Neuroscience, 253, 398–405. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.09.001.

Barreiro, K. A, Suárez, L. D., Lynch, V. M., Molina, V. A., & Delorenzi, A. (2013). Memory expression is independent of memory labilization/ reconsolidation. Neurobiology of Learning & Memory, 106, 283–291. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2013.10.006

Boccia, M. M., Blake, M. G., Acosta, G. B., & Baratti, C. M. (2006). Post-retrieval effects of ICV infusions of hemicholinium in mice are dependent on the age of the original memory. Learning & Memory, 13, 376–381. doi:10.1101/lm.150306

Bos, M. G. N., Beckers, T., & Kindt, M. (2014). Noradrenergic blockade of memory reconsolidation: A failure to reduce conditioned fear responding. Frontiers of Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 1–8. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00412

Bouton, M. E. (2004). Context and behavioral processes in extinction. Learning & Memory, 11, 485– 494.

Brunet, A., Poundja, J., Tremblay, J., Bui, E., Thomas, E., Orr, S. P., . . . Pitman, R. K. (2011). Trauma reactivation under the influence of propranolol decreases posttraumatic stress symptoms and disorder: Three open-label trials. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 31, 547–550. doi:10.1097/ JCP.0b013e318222f360

Caffaro, P. A., Suarez, L. D., Blake, M. G., & Delorenzi, A. (2012). Dissociation between memory reactivation and its behavioral expression: Scopolamine interferes with memory expression without disrupting long-term storage. Neurobiology of Learning & Memory, 98, 235–245. doi:10.1016/j. nlm.2012.08.003

Cammarota, M., Bevilaqua, L. R. M., Medina, J. H., & Izquierdo, I. (2004). Retrieval does not induce reconsolidation of inhibitory avoidance memory. Learning & Memory, 11,  572–578. doi:10.1101/ lm.76804

Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 181–204. doi:10.1017/S0140525X12000477

Clem, R. L., & Huganir, R. L. (2010). Calcium-permeable AMPA receptor dynamics mediate fear memory erasure. Science, 330, 1108–1112. doi:10.1126/ science.1195298

Coccoz, V., Maldonado, H., & Delorenzi, A. (2011). The enhancement of reconsolidation with a naturalistic mild stressor improves the expression of a declarative memory in humans. Neuroscience, 185, 61–72. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.04.023

de-Wit, L., Machilsen, B., & Putzeys, T. (2010). Predictive coding and the neural response to predictable stimuli. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 8702– 8703. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.2248-10.2010

Debiec, J., Díaz-Mataix, L., Bush, D. E. A., Doyère, V., & LeDoux, J. E. (2010). The amygdala encodes specific sensory features of an aversive reinforcer. Nature Neuroscience, 13, 536–537. doi:10.1038/ nn.2520

Debiec, J., Díaz-Mataix, L., Bush, D. E. A., Doyère, V., & LeDoux, J. E. (2013). The selectivity of aversive memory reconsolidation and extinction processes depends on the initial encoding of the Pavlovian association. Learning & Memory, 20, 695–699. doi:10.1101/lm.031609.113

Debiec, J., Doyère, V., Nader, K., & LeDoux, J. E. (2006). Directly reactivated, but not indirectly reactivated, memories undergo reconsolidation in the amygdala. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103, 3428–3433. doi:10.1073/pnas.0507168103

Debiec, J., LeDoux, J. E., & Nader, K. (2002). Cellular and systems reconsolidation in the hippocampus. Neuron, 36, 527–538. doi:10.1016/S0896- 6273(02)01001-2

Delorenzi, A., Maza, F. J., Suárez, L. D., Barreiro, K., Molina, V. A., & Stehberg, J. (2014). Memory beyond expression. Journal of Physiology (Paris), 108, 307–322. doi:10.1016/j.jphysparis.2014.07.002

Díaz-Mataix, L., Debiec, J., LeDoux, J. E., & Doyère, V. (2011). Sensory specific associations stored in the lateral amygdala allow for selective alteration of fear memories. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 9538–9543. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.5808-10.2011

Díaz-Mataix, L., Ruiz Martinez, R. C., Schafe, G. E., LeDoux, J. E., & Doyère, V. (2013). Detection of a temporal error triggers reconsolidation of amygdala-dependent memories. Current Biology, 23, 1–6. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.053 Duvarci, S., Mamou, C. S., & Nader, K. (2006). Extinction is not a sufficient condition to prevent fear memories from undergoing reconsolidation in the basolateral amygdala. European Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 249–260. doi:10.1111/j.1460- 9568.2006.04907.x

Duvarci, S., & Nader, K. (2004). Characterization of fear memory reconsolidation. The Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 9269–9275. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.2971-04.2004

Ecker, B. (2003, November). The hidden logic of anxiety: Look for the emotional truth behind the symptom. Psychotherapy Networker, 27(6), 38–43, 58.

Ecker, B. (2006, July). The effectiveness of psychotherapy. Keynote address, 12th Biennial Conference of the Constructivist Psychology Network, University of California, San Marcos. Transcript: www.coherencetherapy.org/files/ecker2006cpnkeynote.pdf

Ecker, B. (2008, September). Unlocking the emotional brain: Finding the neural key to transformation. Psychotherapy Networker, 32(5), 42–47, 60.

Unlocking the Emotional Brain: Eliminating Symptoms at Their Roots Using Memory Reconsolidation

Ecker, B. (2010, January). The brain’s rules for change: Translating cutting-edge neuroscience into practice. Psychotherapy Networker, 34(1), 43–45, 60.

Ecker, B. (2011, January 13). Reconsolidation: A universal, integrative framework for highly effective psychotherapy [Web log post]. Retrieved November 15, 2014, from http://bit.ly/1zjKtMr

Ecker, B. (2013, May). Unlocking the emotional brain: Memory reconsolidation, therapeutic effectiveness and the further evolution of psychotherapy. Keynote address, 49th Annual Conference of the California Association of Marriage and Family Therapists, Sacramento, California.

Ecker, B. (2014, July 14). Annals of memory reconsolidation: Lagging accounts cause confusion [Web log post]. Retrieved from  http://www.neuropsychotherapist.com/annals-ofmemory-reconsolidation-lagging-accounts-causeconfusion/

Ecker, B. (2015). Using NLP for memory reconsolidation: A glimpse of integrating the panoply of psychotherapies. The Neuropsychotherapist, 10, 50–56. doi:10.12744/tnpt(10)050-056

Ecker, B., & Hulley, L. (1996). Depth oriented brief therapy: How to be brief when you were trained to be deep, and vice versa. San Francisco, CA: JosseyBass.

Ecker, B., & Hulley, L. (2000). The order in clinical “disorder”: Symptom coherence in depth oriented brief therapy. In R. A. Neimeyer & J. D. Raskin (Eds.), Constructions of disorder: Meaning-making frameworks for psychotherapy (pp. 63–89). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Press.

Ecker, B., & Hulley, L. (2011). Coherence therapy practice manual and training guide. Oakland, CA: Coherence Psychology Institute. Online: www.coherencetherapy.org/ resources/manual.htm

Ecker, B., Hulley, L., & Ticic, R. (in press). Minding the findings: Let’s not miss the message of memory reconsolidation research for psychotherapy. Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

Ecker, B., Ticic, R., & Hulley, L. (2012). Unlocking the emotional brain: Eliminating symptoms at their roots using memory reconsolidation. New York, NY: Routledge.

Ecker, B., Ticic, R., & Hulley, L. (2013a, April). A primer on memory reconsolidation and its psychotherapeutic use as a core process of profound change. The Neuropsychotherapist, 1, 82–99. doi:10.12744/ tnpt(1)082-099 Ecker, B., Ticic, R., & Hulley, L. (2013b, July). Unlocking the emotional brain: Is memory reconsolidation the key to transformation? Psychotherapy Networker, 37(4), 18–25, 46–47.

Ecker, B., & Toomey, B. (2008). Depotentiation of symptom-producing implicit memory in coherence therapy. Journal of Constructivist Psychology, 21, 87–150.

Eisenberg, M., & Dudai, Y. (2004). Reconsolidation of fresh, remote, and extinguished fear memory in Medaka: Old fears don’t die. European Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 3397–3403. doi:10.1111/j.1460- 9568.2004.03818.x

Eisenberg, M., Kobilo, T., Berman, D. E., & Dudai, Y. (2003). Stability of retrieved memory: Inverse correlation with trace dominance. Science, 301, 1102– 1104. doi:10.1126/science.1086881

Exton-McGuinness, M. T. J., Lee, J. L. C., & Reichelt, A. C. (2015). Updating memories: The role of prediction errors in memory reconsolidation. Behavioural Brain Research, 278, 375–384. doi:10.1016/j. bbr.2014.10.011

Exton-McGuinness, M. T. J., Patton, R. C., Sacco, L. B., & Lee, J. L. C. (2014). Reconsolidation of a welllearned instrumental memory. Learning & Memory, 21, 468–477. doi:10.1101/lm.035543.114

Flavell, C. R., & Lee, J. L. C. (2013). Reconsolidation and extinction of an appetitive pavlovian memory. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory,104, 25– 31. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2013.04.009

Foa, E. B., & McNally, R. J. (1996). Mechanisms of change in exposure therapy. In R. M. Rapee (Ed.), Current controversies in the anxiety disorders (pp. 329–343). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Forcato, C., Argibay, P. F., Pedreira, M. E., & Maldonado, H. (2009). Human reconsolidation does not always occur when a memory is retrieved: The relevance of the reminder structure. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 91, 50–57. doi:10.1016/j. nlm.2008.09.011

Forcato, C., Burgos, V. L., Argibay, P. F., Molina, V. A., Pedreira, M. E., & Maldonado, H. (2007). Reconsolidation of declarative memory in humans. Learning & Memory, 14, 295–303. doi:10.1101/ lm.486107

Forcato, C., Fernandeza, R. S., & Pedreira, M. E. (2014). Strengthening a consolidated memory: The key role of the reconsolidation process. Journal of Physiology (Paris), 108, 323–333. doi:10.1016/j. jphysparis.2014.09.001

Forcato, C., Rodríguez, M. L. C., Pedreira, M. E., & Maldonado, H. (2010). Reconsolidation in humans opens up declarative memory to the entrance of new information. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 93, 77–84. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2009.08.006

Frankland, P. W., Ding, H. K., Takahashi, E., Suzuki, A., Kida, S., & Silva, A. J. (2006). Stability of recent and remote contextual fear memory. Learning & Memory, 13, 451–457. doi:10.1101/lm.183406

Frenkel, L., Maldonado, H., & Delorenzi, A. (2005). Memory strengthening by a real-life episode during reconsolidation: An outcome of water deprivation via brain angiotensin II. European Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 1757–1766. doi:10.1111/j.1460- 9568.2005.04373.x

Friston, K. (2010). The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 127–138. doi:10.1038/nrn2787

Galluccio, L. (2005). Updating reactivated memories in infancy: I. Passive- and active-exposure effects. Developmental Psychobiology, 47, 1–17. doi:10.1002/dev.20073

Gray, R. M., & Liotta, R. F. (2012). PTSD: Extinction, reconsolidation, and the visual-kinesthetic dissociation protocol. Traumatology, 18, 3–16. doi:10.1177/1534765611431835

Hernandez. P. J., & Kelley, A. E. (2004). Long-term memory for instrumental responses does not undergo protein synthesis-dependent reconsolidation upon retrieval. Learning & Memory, 11, 748–754. doi:10.1101/lm.84904

Högberg, G., Nardo, D., Hällström, T., & Pagani, M. (2011). Affective psychotherapy in post-traumatic reactions guided by affective neuroscience: Memory reconsolidation and play. Psychology Research and Behavior Management, 4, 87–96. doi:10.2147/ PRBM.S10380

Hupbach, A., Gomez, R., Hardt, O., & Nadel, L. (2007). Reconsolidation of episodic memories: A subtle reminder triggers integration of new information. Learning & Memory, 14, 47–53. doi:10.1101/ lm.365707

Hupbach, A., Gomez, R., & Nadel, L. (2009). Episodic memory reconsolidation: Updating or source confusion? Memory, 17, 502–510. doi:10.1080/09658210902882399

Inda, M. C., Muravieva, E. V., & Alberini, C. M. (2011). Memory retrieval and the passage of time: From reconsolidation and strengthening to extinction. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 1635–1643. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.4736-10.2011

Jacobs, N. S., Allen, T. A., Nguyen, N., & Fortin, N. J.(2013). Critical role of the hippocampus in memory for elapsed time. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 13888–13893. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.1733-13.2013

Jarome, T. J., Kwapis, J. L., Werner, C. T., Parsons, R. G., Gafford, G. M., & Helmstetter, F. J. (2012). The timing of multiple retrieval events can alter GluR1 phosphorylation and the requirement for protein synthesis in fear memory reconsolidation. Learning & Memory, 19, 300–306. doi:10.1101/ lm.024901.111

Judge, M. E., & Quartermain D. (1982). Alleviation of anisomycin-induced amnesia by pre-test treatment with lysine-vasopressin. Pharmacology Biochemistry & Behavior, 16, 463–466. doi:10.1016/0091- 3057(82)90453-1

Kindt, M., Soeter, M., & Vervliet, B. (2009). Beyond extinction: Erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear. Nature Neuroscience, 12, 256–258. doi:10.1038/nn.2271

Kirtley, A., & Thomas, K. L. (2010). The exclusive induction of extinction is gated by BDNF. Learning & Memory, 17, 612–619. doi:10.1101/lm.1877010

Lane, R. D., Ryan, L., Nadel, L., & Greenberg, L. (in press). Memory reconsolidation, emotional arousal and the process of change in psychotherapy: New insights from brain science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. doi:10.1017/S0140525X14000041.

LeDoux, J. E., Romanski, L., & Xagoraris, A. (1989). Indelibility of subcortical emotional memories. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1, 238–243. doi:10.1162/jocn.1989.1.3.238 Lee, C. W., Taylor, G., & Drummond, P. D. (2006). The active ingredient in EMDR: Is it traditional exposure or dual focus of attention? Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 13, 97–107. doi:10.1002/ cpp.479

Lee, J. L. (2009). Reconsolidation: Maintaining memory relevance. Trends in Neuroscience, 32, 413–420. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2009.05.002

Lee, J. L., Milton, A. L., & Everitt, B. J. (2006). Reconsolidation and extinction of conditioned fear: Inhibition and potentiation. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 10051–10056. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.2466-06.2006

Lee, S. H., Choi, J. H., Lee, N., Lee, H. R., Kim, J. I., Yu, N. K., . . . Kaang, B. K. (2008). Synaptic protein degradation underlies destabilization of retrieved fear memory. Science, 319, 1253–1256. doi:10.1126/science.1150541

Lewis, D. J. (1979). Psychobiology of active and inactive memory. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 1054–1083.

Lewis, D., Bregman, N. J., & Mahan, J. (1972). Cuedependent amnesia in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 81, 243–247. doi:10.1037/h0033524

Lewis, D. J., & Bregman, N. J. (1973). Source of cues for cue-dependent amnesia in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 85, 421–426. doi:10.1037/h0035020

Liu, J., Zhao, L., Xue, Y., Shi, J., Suo, L., Luo, Y., . . . Lu, L. (2014). An unconditioned stimulus retrieval extinction procedure to prevent the return of fear memory. Biological Psychiatry, 76, 895–901. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.03.027

MacDonald, C. J., Lepage, K. Q., Eden, U. T., & Eichenbaum, H. (2011). Hippocampal “time cells” bridge the gap in memory for discontiguous events. Neuron, 71, 737–749. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.07.012

Mactutus, C. F., Riccio, D. C., & Ferek, J. M. (1979). Retrograde amnesia for old (reactivated) memory: Some anomalous characteristics. Science, 204, 1319–1320. doi:10.1126/science.572083

Mamiya, N., Fukushima, H., Suzuki, A., Matsuyama, Z., Homma, S., Frankland, P. W., & Kida, S. (2009). Brain region-specific gene expression activation required for reconsolidation and extinction of contextual fear memory. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 402–413. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.4639-08.2009

McGaugh, J. L. (1989). Involvement of hormonal and neuromodulatory systems in the regulation of memory storage. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 2, 255– 287. doi:10.1146/annurev.ne.12.030189.001351

McGaugh, J. L. (2000). Memory: A century of consolidation. Science, 287, 248–251. doi:10.1126/science.287.5451.248

McGaugh, J. L., & Roozendaal, B. (2002). Role of adrenal stress hormones in forming lasting memories in the brain. Current Opinions in Neurobiology, 12, 205–210. doi:10.1016/s0959-4388(02)00306-9

Merlo, E., Milton, A. L., Goozée, Z. Y., Theobald, D. E., & Everitt, B. J. (2014). Reconsolidation and extinction are dissociable and mutually exclusive processes: Behavioral and molecular evidence. The Journal of Neuroscience, 34, 2422–2431. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.4001-13.2014

Milad, M. R., & Quirk, J. G. (2002). Neurons in medial prefrontal cortex signal memory for fear extinction. Nature, 420, 70–74. doi:10.1038/nature01138

Milekic, M. H., & Alberini, C. M. (2002). Temporally graded requirement for protein synthesis following memory reactivation. Neuron, 36, 521–525. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00976-5

Mileusnic, R., Lancashire, C. L., & Rose, S. P. R. (2005). Recalling an aversive experience by day-old chicks is not dependent on somatic protein synthesis. Learning & Memory, 12, 615–619. doi:10.1101/ lm.38005

Milner, B., Squire, L. R., & Kandel, E. R. (1998). Cognitive neuroscience and the study of memory. Neuron, 20, 445–468. doi:10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80987-3

Misanin, J. R., Miller, R. R., & Lewis, D. J. (1968). Retrograde amnesia produced by electroconvulsive shock following reactivation of a consolidated memory trace. Science, 16, 554–555. doi:10.1126/ science.160.3827.554

Monfils, M.-H., Cowansage, K. K., Klann, E., & LeDoux, J. E. (2009). Extinction-reconsolidation boundaries: Key to persistent attenuation of fear memories. Science, 324, 951–955. doi:10.1126/science.1167975

Morris, R. G., Inglis, J., Ainge, J. A., Olverman, H. J., Tulloch, J., Dudai, Y., & Kelly, P. A. (2006). Memory reconsolidation: Sensitivity of spatial memory to inhibition of protein synthesis in dorsal hippocampus during encoding and retrieval. Neuron, 50, 479–489. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2006.04.012

Myers, K. M., & Davis, M. (2002). Behavioral and neural analysis of extinction. Neuron, 36, 567–584. Nader, K. (2003). Memory traces unbound. Trends in Neurosciences, 26, 65–72. doi:10.1016/s0166- 2236(02)00042-5

Nader, K., Schafe, G. E., & LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Fear memories require protein synthesis in the amygdala for reconsolidation after retrieval. Nature, 406, 722–726. doi:10.1038/35021052

Naya, Y., & Suzuki, W. A. (2011). Integrating what and when across the primate medial temporal lobe. Science, 333, 773–776. doi:10.1126/science.1206773

Paz, R., Gelbard-Sagiv, H., Mukamel, R., Harel, M., Malach, R., & Fried, I. (2010). A neural substrate in the human hippocampus for linking successive events. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 6046–6051. doi:10.1073/ pnas.0910834107

Pedreira, M. E., & Maldonado, H. (2003). Protein synthesis subserves reconsolidation or extinction depending on reminder duration. Neuron, 38, 863– 869. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00352-0

Pedreira, M. E., Pérez-Cuesta, L. M., & Maldonado, H. (2002). Reactivation and reconsolidation of longterm memory in the crab Chasmagnathus: Protein synthesis requirement and mediation by NMDAtype glutamatergic receptors. The Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 8305–8311.

Pedreira, M. E., Pérez-Cuesta, L. M., & Maldonado, H. (2004). Mismatch between what is expected and what actually occurs triggers memory reconsolidation or extinction. Learning & Memory, 11, 579–585. doi:10.1101/lm.76904

Pérez-Cuesta, L. M., & Maldonado, H. (2009). Memory reconsolidation and extinction in the crab: Mutual exclusion or coexistence? Learning & Memory, 16, 714–721. doi:10.1101/lm.1544609

Phelps, E. A., Delgado, M. R., Nearing, K. I., & LeDoux, J. E. (2004). Extinction learning in humans: Role of the amygdala and vmPFC. Neuron, 43, 897–905. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.042

Piaget, J. (1955). The child’s construction of reality. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

Pine, A., Mendelsohn, A., & Dudai, Y. (2014). Unconscious learning of likes and dislikes is persistent, resilient, and reconsolidates. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(1051), 1–13. doi:10.3389/ fpsyg.2014.01051

Pittenger, D. J., & Pavlik, W. B. (1988). Analysis of the partial reinforcement extinction effect in humans using absolute and relative comparisons of schedules. American Journal of Psychology, 101, 1–14.

Przybyslawski, J., Roullet, P., & Sara, S. J. (1999). Attenuation of emotional and nonemotional memories after their reactivation: Role of beta adrenergic receptors. The Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 6623– 6628.

Przybyslawski, J., & Sara, S. J. (1997). Reconsolidation of memory after its reactivation. Behavioural Brain Research, 84, 241–246. doi:10.1016/S0166- 4328(96)00153-2

Quirk, G. J., Likhtik, E., Pelletier, J. G., & Pare, D. (2003). Stimulation of medial prefrontal cortex decreases the responsiveness of central amygdala output neurons. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 8800–8807.

Quirk, G. J., Paré, D., Richardson, R., Herry, C., Monfils, M. H., Schiller, D., & Vicentic, A. (2010). Erasing fear memories with extinction training. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 14993–14997. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.4268-10.2010

Reichelt, A. C., & Lee, J. L. C. (2013). Memory reconsolidation in aversive and appetitive settings. Frontiers of Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 1–18. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00118

Reichelt, A. C., Exton-McGuinness, M. T., & Lee, J. L. (2013). Ventral tegmental dopamine dysregulation prevents appetitive memory destabilisation. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 14205–14210. doi:10.1523/ jneurosci.1614-13.2013

Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In A. H. Prokasy (Ed.), Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory (pp. 64–99). New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Richardson, R., Riccio, D. C., & Mowrey, H. (1982). Retrograde amnesia for previously acquired Pavlovian conditioning: UCS exposure as a reactivation treatment. Physiology of Psychology, 10, 384–390.

Rodriguez-Ortiz, C. J., De la Cruz, V., Gutierrez, R., & Bermidez-Rattoni, F. (2005). Protein synthesis underlies post-retrieval memory consolidation to a restricted degree only when updated information is obtained. Learning & Memory, 12, 533–537. doi:10.1101/lm.94505

Rodriguez-Ortiz, C. J., Garcia-DeLaTorre, P., Benavidez, E., Ballesteros, M. A., & Bermudez-Rattoni, F. (2008). Intrahippocampal anisomycin infusions disrupt previously consolidated spatial memory only when memory is updated. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 89, 352–359. doi:10.1016/j. nlm.2007.10.004

Roozendaal, B., McEwen, B. S., & Chattarji, S. (2009). Stress, memory and the amygdala. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 423–433. doi:10.1038/nrn2651

Rossato, J. I., Bevilaqua, L. R. M., Medina, J. H., Izquierdo, I., & Cammarota, M. (2006). Retrieval induces hippocampal-dependent reconsolidation of spatial memory. Learning & Memory, 13, 431–440. doi:10.1101/lm.315206

Rossato, J. I., Bevilaqua, L. R. M., Myskiw, J. C., Medina, J. H., Izquierdo, I., & Cammarota, M. (2007). On the role of hippocampal protein synthesis in the consolidation and reconsolidation of object recognition memory. Learning & Memory, 14, 36–46. doi:10.1101/lm.422607

Roullet, P., & Sara, S. J. (1998). Consolidation of memory after its reactivation: Involvement of ß noradrenergic receptors in the late phase. Neural Plasticity, 6, 63–68. doi:10.1155/np.1998.63

Rubin, R. D. (1976). Clinical use of retrograde amnesia produced by electroconvulsive shock: A conditioning hypothesis. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 21, 87–90.

Rubin, R. D., Fried, R., & Franks, C. M. (1969). New application of ECT. In R. D. Rubin & C. Franks (Eds.), Advances in behavior therapy, 1968 (pp. 37– 44). New York, NY: Academic Press.

Santini, E., Ge, H., Ren, K., de Ortiz, S. P., & Quirk, G. J. (2004). Consolidation of fear extinction requires protein synthesis in the medial prefrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 5704–5710. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.0786-04.2004

Santoyo-Zedillo, M., Rodriguez-Ortiz, C. J., ChavezMarchetta, G., Bermudez-Rattoni, F., & Balderas, I. (2014). Retrieval is not necessary to trigger reconsolidation of object recognition memory in the perirhinal cortex. Learning & Memory, 21, 452–456. doi:10.1101/lm.035428.114

Sara, S. J. (2000). Retrieval and reconsolidation: Toward a neurobiology of remembering. Learning & Memory, 7, 73–84. doi:10.1101/lm.7.2.73

Schiller, D., Monfils, M.-H., Raio, C. M., Johnson, D. C., LeDoux, J. E., & Phelps, E. A. (2010). Preventing the return of fear in humans using reconsolidation update mechanisms. Nature, 463, 49–53. doi:10.1038/nature08637

Schiller, D., & Phelps, E. A. (2011). Does reconsolidation occur in humans? Frontiers of Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 1–18. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2011.00024

Schore, A. N. (2003). Affect dysregulation and disorders of the self. New York, NY: W.W. Norton.

Sekiguchi, T., Yamada, A., & Suzuki, H. (1997). Reactivation-dependent changes in memory states in the terrestrial slug Limax flavus. Learning & Memory, 4, 356–364. doi:10.1101/lm.4.4.356

Sevenster, D., Beckers, T., & Kindt, M. (2012). Retrieval per se is not sufficient to trigger reconsolidation of human fear memory. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 97, 338–345. doi:10.1016/j. nlm.2012.01.009

Sevenster, D., Beckers, T., & Kindt, M. (2013). Prediction error governs pharmacologically induced amnesia for learned fear. Science, 339, 830–833. doi:10.1126/science.1231357

Sevenster, D., Beckers, T., & Kindt, M. (2014). Prediction error demarcates the transition from retrieval,to reconsolidation, to new learning. Learning & Memory, 21, 580–584. doi:10.1101/lm.035493.114

Sibson, P. & Ticic, R. (2014, March). Remembering in order to forget. Therapy Today, 25(2), 26–29.

Siegel, D. J. (2006). An interpersonal neurobiology approach to psychotherapy. Psychiatric Annals, 36, 248–258.

Soeter, M., & Kindt, M. (2011). Disrupting reconsolidation: Pharmacological and behavioral manipulations. Learning & Memory, 18, 357–366. doi:10.1101/lm.2148511

Steinfurth, E. C. K., Kanen, J. W., Raio, C. M., Clem, R. L., Huganir, R. L., & Phelps, E. A. (2014). Young and old Pavlovian fear memories can be modified with extinction training during reconsolidation in humans. Learning & Memory, 21, 338–341. doi:10.1101/lm.033589.113

Stollhoff, N., Menzel, R., & Eisenhardt, D. (2005). Spontaneous recovery from extinction depends on the reconsolidation of the acquisition memory in an appetitive learning paradigm in the honeybee (Apis mellifera). The Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 4485–4492. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.0117-05.2005

Suzuki, A., Josselyn, S. A., Frankland, P. W., Masushige, S., Silva, A. J., & Kida, S. (2004). Memory reconsolidation and extinction have distinct temporal and biochemical signatures. The Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 4787–4795. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.5491-03.2004

Toomey, B., & Ecker, B. (2007). Of neurons and knowings: Constructivism, coherence psychology and their neurodynamic substrates. Journal of Constructivist Psychology, 20, 201–245. doi:10.1080/10720530701347860

Toomey, B., & Ecker, B. (2009). Competing visions of the implications of neuroscience for psychotherapy. Journal of Constructivist Psychology, 22, 95–140. doi:10.1080/10720530802675748 Tronson, N. C., & Taylor, J. R. (2007). Molecular mechanisms of memory reconsolidation. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 262–275. doi:10.1038/nrn2090

van der Kolk, B. (1994). The body keeps the score: Memory and the evolving psychobiology of posttraumatic stress. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 1, 253–265. doi:10.3109/10673229409017088

Walker, M. P., Brakefield, T., Hobson, J. A., & Stickgold, R. (2003). Dissociable stages of human memory consolidation and reconsolidation. Nature, 425, 616–620. doi:10.1038/nature01930

Winters, B. D., Tucci, M. C., & DaCosta-Furtado, M. (2009). Older and stronger object memories are selectively destabilized by reactivation in the presence of new information. Learning & Memory, 16, 545–553. doi:10.1101/lm.1509909

Wood, N. E., Rosasco, M. L., Suris, A. M., Spring, J. D., Marin, M.-F., Lasko, N. B., . . . Pitman, R. K. (2015). Pharmacological blockade of memory reconsolidation in posttraumatic stress disorder: Three negative psychophysiological studies. Psychiatry Research, 225, 31–39. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2014.09.005

Xue, Y.-X., Luo, Y.-X., Wu, P., Shi, H.-S., Xue, L.-F., Chen, C., . . . Lu, L. (2012). A memory retrievalextinction procedure to prevent drug craving and relapse. Science, 336, 241–245. doi:10.1126/science.1215070

Excerpt from International Journal of Neuropsychotherapy Volume 3 Issue 1 (2015)

0:00
0:00